Details
Title
Forest before illusory trees: illusory contours of local level elements do not influence perceptual global advantage in the hierarchical structure processingJournal title
Polish Psychological BulletinYearbook
2015Issue
No 4Authors
Divisions of PAS
Nauki Humanistyczne i SpołecznePublisher
Committee for Psychological Science PASDate
2015[2015.01.01 AD - 2015.12.31 AD]Identifier
DOI: 10.1515/ppb-2015-0070Source
Polish Psychological Bulletin; 2015; No 4References
Gillam (2002), Subjective contours at line terminations depend on scene layout analysis , not image processing of : and, Journal Experimental Psychology Human Perception Performance, 28, 43, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.28.1.43 ; Navon (2003), What does a compound letter tell the psychologist s mind, Acta Psychologica, 114, doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.06.002 ; Martin (1979), Local and global processing : The role of sparsity and, Memory Cognition, 7, 476, doi.org/10.3758/BF03198264 ; Koivisto (2004), Preconscious analysis of global structure : Evidence from masked priming, Visual Cognition, 11, 105, doi.org/10.1080/13506280344000266 ; Spillmann (1995), Phenomena of illusory form : can we bridge between levels of explanation, Perception, 24, 1333, doi.org/10.1068/p241333 ; Miller (1981), Global precedence in attention and decision of : and, Journal Experimental Psychology Human Perception Performance, 7, 1161, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.7.6.1161 ; Kinchla (1979), The order of visual processing : Topdown , bottom - up , or middle out, Perception Psychophysics, 25, 225, doi.org/10.3758/BF03202991 ; Murray (2013), Illusory contours : a window onto the neurophysiology of constructing perception in, Trends Cognitive Sciences, 17, 471, doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.07.004 ; Lamb (1988), The processing of hierarchical stimuli : Effects of retinal locus , locational uncertainty , and stimulus identity, Perception Psychophysics, 44, 172, doi.org/10.3758/BF03208710 ; Shulman (1986), The role of spatial - frequency channels in the perception of local and global structure, Perception, 15, 259, doi.org/10.1068/p150259 ; Fink (1997), Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual stimuli, Brain, 120, 1779, doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.10.1779 ; Han (1999), Parallel and competitive processes in hierarchical analysis : perceptual grouping and encoding of closure of : and, Journal Experimental Psychology Human Perception Performance, 25, 1411, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.25.5.1411 ; LaGasse (1993), Effects of good form and spatial frequency on global precedence, Perception Psychophysics, 53, doi.org/10.3758/BF03211718 ; Boer (1982), Global precedence as a postperceptual effect : An analysis of speed - accuracy tradeoff functions, Perception Psychophysics, 13, 358, doi.org/10.3758/BF03202660 ; Han (1997), On the different processing of wholes and parts : A psychophyiological analysis of, Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 687, doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.687 ; Hughes (1996), Global precedence , spatial frequency channels , and the statistic of the natural image of, Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 197, doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.3.197 ; Robertson (1996), Attentional persistence for features of hierarchical patterns of :, Journal Experimental Psychology General, 125, doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.125.3.227 ; Hughes (1984), Global precedence in visual pattern recognition, Perception Psychophysics, 35, 361, doi.org/10.3758/BF03206340 ; Grice (1983), Forest before trees ? It depends where you look, Perception Psychophysics, 33, 121, doi.org/10.3758/BF03202829 ; Hubner (2005), The integration of object levels and their content : a theory of global / local processing and related hemispheric differences of and, Journal Experimental Psychology Human Perception Performance, 31, 520, doi.org/10.3758/BF03193762 ; Shipley (1992), Strength of visual interpolation depends on the ratio of physically specifi ed to total edge length, Perception Psychophysics, 52, doi.org/10.3758/BF03206762 ; Kinchla (1983), Attending to different levels of structure in a visual image, Perception Psychophysics, 25, 225, doi.org/10.3758/BF03205860 ; Miller (2002), Global precedence and response activation : evidence from LRPs of Human, The Quarterly Journal Experimental Psychology Experimental Psychology, 55, 289, doi.org/10.1080/02724980143000280 ; Ehrenstein (1941), Über Abwandlungen der L Hermannschen Helligkeitserscheinung für, Zeitschrift Psychologie, 150. ; Di Lollo (2001), The preattentive emperor has no clothes : A dynamic redressing of :, Journal Experimental Psychology General, 130, doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.479 ; Sebrechts (1985), Variation on parts and wholes : Information precedence vs global precedence Proceedings of the Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive, Science Society, 11. ; Altschuler (2012), Early Electrophysiological indices of illusory contour processing within the lateral occipital complex are virtually impervious to large - scale manipulations of illusion strength, NeuroImage, 59, doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.051 ; Navon (1981), The forest revisited : More on global precedence, Psychological Research, 43, 1, doi.org/10.1007/BF00309635 ; Salvano (2006), Can subthreshold summation be observed in the Ehrenstein illusion, Perception, 35, 965, doi.org/10.1080/13506280902949312 ; Conci (2007), The contrasting impact of global and local object attributes on Kanizsa fi gure detection, Perception Psychophysics, 69, doi.org/10.3758/BF03192945 ; Luna (1993), Effects of exposure duration and eccentricity of global and local information on processing dominance of, European Journal Cognitive Psychology, 5, 183, doi.org/10.1080/09541449308520115 ; Hoffman (1980), Interaction between global and local levels of a form of : and, Journal Experimental Psychology Human Perception Performance, 6, 222, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.6.2.222 ; Lamb (1996), Spatial frequency and attention : Effects of level - , target - , and location - repetition on the processing of global and local forms, Perception Psychophysics, 58, 363, doi.org/10.3758/BF03206812 ; Kimchi (2004), Perceptual grouping and attention : Not all groupings are equal, Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 11, 687, doi.org/10.3758/BF03196621 ; Heinze (1993), Electrophysiological correlates of hierarchical stimulus processing : dissociation between onset and later stages of global and local target processing, Neuropsychologia, 31, 841, doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(93)90132-J ; Bruyer (2003), Aging and the locus of the global precedence effect : A short review and new empirical data, Experimental Aging Research, 29, 237, doi.org/10.1080/03610730303724 ; Kimchi (1992), The primacy of wholistic processing and global / local paradigm : A critical review, Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24, doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.24 ; Kimchi (1998), Uniform connectedness and grouping in the perceptual organization of hierarchical patterns of Experimental Psy chology : Human Perception and Performance, Journal, 24, 1105, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.4.1105 ; Kanizsa (1974), Contours without gradients or cognitive contours of, Italian Journal Psychology, 1. ; Lesher (1993), The role of edges and line - ends in illusory contour formation, Vision Research, 33, 2253, doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(93)90104-5 ; Palmer (1995), Local - level and global - level form characteristics in apparent - motion correspondence, Perception, 24, 1233, doi.org/10.1068/p241233 ; Mevorach (2006), Effects of saliency , not global dominance , in patients with left parietal damage, Neuropsychologia, 44, 307, doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.015 ; Enns (1995), Access to global and local properties in visual search for compound stimuli, Psychological Science, 6, 283, doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00512.x ; Lamb (2000), Contributions of automatic and controlled processes to the analysis of hierarchical structure of : and, Journal Experimental Psychology Human Perception Performance, 26, 234, doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.26.1.234 ; Navon (1977), Forest before trees : The precedence of global features in visual perception, Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353, doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3 ; Kimchi (1991), Hemispheric processing of global form , local form , and texture, Acta Psychologica, 76, 133, doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(91)90042-X ; Hughes (1990), Global versus local processing in the absence of low spatial frequencies of, Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 272, doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1990.2.3.272 ; Han (2000), Interactions between spatial and global / local feature selection : An ERP study, Neuroreport, 11, 2753, doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200008210-00029 ; Conci (2011), The timecourse of global and local attentional guidance in Kanizsa - fi gure detection, Neuropsychologia, 49, 2456, doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.023 ; Salvano (2000), Spatial factors of brightness illusion in the Ehrenstein fi gure, Perception, 29, 709, doi.org/10.1068/p2922 ; Andres (2006), Effect of short and long exposure duration and dualtasking on a global - local task, Acta Psychologica, 122, doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.12.002 ; Poirel (2008), What does the nature of the stimuli tell us about the Global Precedence Effect, Acta Psychologica, 127, doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.12.001 ; Robertson (1993), Interhemispheric relations in processing hierarchical patterns : Evidence from normal and commissurotomized subjects, Neuropsychology, 7, 325, doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.7.3.325 ; Weissman (2005), Hemispheric asymmetries for different components of global / local attention occur in distinct temporo - parietal loci, Cerebral Cortex, 15, 870, doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh187 ; Paquet (1984), Global precedence : The effect of exposure duration of, Canadian Journal Psychology, 38, doi.org/10.1037/h0080783 ; Han (1999), Interactions between perceptual organization based on Gestalt laws and those based on hierarchical processing, Perception Psychophysics, 61, 1287, doi.org/10.3758/BF03206180 ; Badcock (1990), Low - frequency filtering and the process of local - global stimuli, Perception, 19, 617, doi.org/10.1068/p190617 ; Ward (1982), Determinants of attention to local and global features of visual forms of : and, Journal Experimental Psychology Human Perception Performance, 8, 562, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.562Aims and scope
Polish Psychological Bulletin (founded in 1970) is an official journal of Polish Academy of Sciences, Committee for Psychological Science.The journal publish a variety of papers, including empirical reports of experiments, surveys and field studies, theoretical articles, controversies and analytic papers on important psychological topics. Relevance for an international readership is our prominent goal, Polish Psychological Bulletin does not publish clinical case studies, or technical articles. Submissions from all domains of psychology are encouraged, especially those that address new developments and pursue innovative approaches.Periodically, the journal will announce a call for papers for special issues. The journal will also entertain unsolicited proposals for special issues that fit the stated scope of the Polish Psychiological Bulletin (please contact the journal’s Editor-in-Chief with a detailed description of your proposal).
All published research articles in this journal have undergone rigorous review, based on initial editor screening and anonymous evaluation of content and merit by independent expert reviewers.
For information on specific requirements, please see the Author Guidelines.
Abstracting & Indexing
Abstracting and Indexing Information
• DESY Publication Database
• Directory of Open Access Journal (DOAJ)
• Current Contents: Social & Behavioral Sciences
• Dimensions
• EBSCO
• ERIH Plus
• Google Scholar
• Index Copernicus
• ProQuest
• PsychArchives
• Science Open
• SCOPUS (Elsevier)
• Sherpa/RoMEO